Skip to main content

Desk Rejects and JETS in Political Science

In the early years of this century, the adherents to the positivistic paradigm, among them the elites of political science, were becoming concerned. The aging protestors from the 1960s revolt against the profession’s silence on the Vietnam War, racism, gender and economic inequality, and other social issues, were attracting too much attention from younger grad students and new PhDs. Fidelity to positivism was fragmenting. There was a rise in the usage of mixed methods and interpretivism.  Criticisms continued about the profession’s fawning efforts to imitate the methods of natural science, like physics, even as awareness grew that human beings and their political behavior are not analogous to either atomic particles or billiard balls in motion. Indeed, one spur of the 60s protests was that, under the guise of “scientific value neutrality,” members of the political science profession were hiring out as strategy consultants to the war mongers and corporate profiteers.*

As the foundation seemed to continue to erode, something had to be done to preserve the dominant paradigm, and the privileges it gave to its elite priests and disciples.

Then came DA-RT and JETS.  The acronym, “DA-RT,” stands for Data Access and Research Transparency. “JETS” means Journal Editor’s Transparency Statement. While a closer look at, and critique of, DA-RT’s positivistic program will be offered in following posts, here it can be said that DA-RT mainly requires data be made available in online repositories, and an explanation given about how it was gathered. Ideally, then, succeeding researchers could use the data to critically examine the research conclusions, analysis, and reasoning of a study. “Replication” is one of their buzz words.

This works fine for some types of research, such as impersonal surveys and the data drawn from them. In this sense, political science methods can imitate natural science. When placebo trials are done, for example, to test the efficacy of a new vaccine, the process is impersonal, and the data can be reported relatively free of bias.

But, the “data” of interpretive studies is different in kind from that of mass surveys and pharmaceutical trials.  In interpretive studies, subjects often tell personal stories. Their openness depends heavily on their trust of the interviewer, and the personal relationship between them. The “data” often consists of the reporter’s empathic interpretation of the subject’s stories. In ethnographic studies, the participant observer intermingles with, and becomes a part of, the group of subjects. Here, the “data” can be hunches, or impressions, observations, and understandings that are personal to the researcher. The “data” can consist of the researcher’s field notes, or recordings taken with the consent of the subjects. Such personal information often cannot be shared without violating trust. 

It is no accident that the kind of research information coming out of interpretivist studies can’t be squeezed into the positivist mold of impersonal methods turning out “objective data.” The DA-RT requirements are intended to preserve the dominance of the positivistic paradigm, and JETS was the signed pledge of editors to enforce the DA-RT program.

By 2010, awareness began to gel in the profession that DA-RT’s doctrines were about to be enacted as the defining principles of the political science profession. These would be the rules for assessing “good work” in political science. Alarmed members of the interpretive/qualitative community aired their protests. A website was organized for objections to be registered. It had dozens of contributors, and thousands of views.

The debates and contributions have been closed off now, and there is only some summaries posted. Was this a surrender? Has the fight over the meaning of political science as a science fizzled out? Have Quals accepted their subordinate place to Quants in political science? I will discuss these questions more in future posts.

By 2016, most of the leading political science journals had taken the JETS pledge. These include the APSR, the American Journal of Political Science, the British Journal of Political Science, the Journal of Politics, the Journal of Conflict Resolution, and others. Comparative Political Studies signed, but later revised their position.

To their credit, there were some heroic editors who refused to sign. These include Jeffry Isaac of Perspective on Politics, and the editors at World Politics, Political Psychology, and The European Journal of Political Research.

The editors who pledged to be the Frontline Defenders of the dominant paradigm, the Positivism Police, would desk reject the clearly non-complying submissions. Papers that seemed to have some compliance would be sent out to like-minded reviewers who also had the power to reject. In most cases, acceptance and publication re-enforced the methodological image which the elites intended to define their profession.

Writing in 2018, the great Interpretivist, Kristen Renwick Monroe, observed that “DA-RT is now widely implemented by JETS signatories.” (Monroe 2018, 141)

How does that implementation work?

Consider the 2019 Editor’s Report on APSR procedures:

In the last quarter of 2018, “28 manuscripts were accepted for publication” by the APSR editors. “With 23 manuscripts most of the acceptances took a quantitative approach, zero manuscripts were interpretative/conceptual, three formal approaches and one qualitative approach.” (Koenig 2019, 586)

I repeat, “one qualitative approach” and 23 quants, out of 28 accepted. In other words, the dominant paradigm had an 82% acceptance rate, while only ONE paper from the competing paradigm was accepted for publication.

What could explain this lopsided result? It seems that the editors for the APSR, and the peer reviewers they invite to review submissions, share the same paradigm. That is, their conceptual framework, which defines “good political science” for them, is itself defined by positivism; i.e., the “quantitative approach.”  With that shared perspective they agree on what looks good, or professional, or scientific. That’s how a dominant paradigm works.

Some women political scientists have argued that DA-RT is biased against women because women are more attracted to qualitative and interpretive research. (Monroe, note 18, 148) Indeed, the Report for APSR states that male submissions outnumber female submissions by more than 3 to 1, and the “share of female authors remains at a low level.” (585) In the 2017-2018 year, 49 published submissions were by males, and only 9 by females. (586)

Scholars who find greater depth and intellectual satisfaction in conducting qualitative and interpretive research are, thus, put at a career disadvantage. Funding, such as from the National Science Foundation, favors “hard science” over interpretive research. Publishing in higher status journals puffs up resumes, and therefore possibilities for career advancement. Thus, following your bliss can result in stalling your career.

A closer look at, and critique of, DA-RT’s positivistic program will be offered in following posts.

 

William J. Kelleher, Ph.D.

@InterpretivePo1 


*One example of the organized opposition to the dominant paradigm in the APSA is, The Caucus for a New Political Science (CNPS – Sec. 27)

About: https://www.cnpsconference.org/about-us

More: https://connect.apsanet.org/s27/

References

Koenig, Thomas , et al. 2019. American Political Science Review Editors’ Report 2017–18.

52 PS: Political Science & Politics (2019), no. 3, 581-587.

DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1049096519000726

Monroe, Kristen Renwick. 2018. The Rush to Transparency: DA-RT and the Potential Dangers for Qualitative Research.

16 Perspectives on Politics (2018), no.1, 141-148.

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S153759271700336X

 


Comments

Popular posts from this blog

How the “But For” Test for Causation in Law can be Adapted to Political Science

For social science, as for law, the concept of “causation” can  take on different forms. These forms of causation are “outside   the box” of the old Newtonian concept of causation. That is, a   one-on-one collision. For example, the “8 ball” in a pool game moved to the corner pocket because the cue ball struck it at the intended angle. The cue ball did that because Minnesota Fats hit the cue ball just right with his pool stick. This is a mechanistic model of causation. In that model, the list of causes prior to Minnesota Fats could go back endlessly; or at least to the Big Bang of 13.8 billion years ago, which, mechanistically, is thought to be the First Cause of everything. However, for the most part, social science, like law, envisions human behavior as conduct for which the actors are responsible. This need not be a “moral” responsibility, for which moral blame is attached. Instead, “causal responsibility” is simply a matter-of-fact, or practical, concept...

Executive Immunity – Beyond the Hype

                                           The media and fund raising emailers are having a ball screaming “the sky is falling! The Trump Supreme Court has given Trump ‘absolute immunity’ for whatever he wants to do as President. He’ll kill us all!” But that’s far from true. The United States v Trump The case is known as US v Trump . In its opinion, the Supreme Court didn’t give Trump anything. In fact, the Court remanded the case to the District Court, and told the lawyers for both sides to start all over again. This time, they have to consider three important points. Before explaining these, lets see how the case got to the High Court. As you probably know, Trump was indicted by a Federal Grand Jury for several criminal charges alleging a conspiracy to change the outcome of the 2020 presidential election. (SCt's Opinion, page 2. All references to the Opinion.) In the...

The Psychology of the Student Protests

University students have been in the news lately because of their protests on campuses and in the streets. Unfortunately, the mainstream media often convey false impressions about the intentions of the protesters. As a result of such misreporting, the meanings to the students of the encampments is widely misperceived. For instance, I recently heard one misinformed person talk about the encampments as if they were a colony of bonobos doing it all day in those tents. As a political scientist who studies all sorts of political behavior, I would like to add a little clarity to our understanding of the mental state, or intentions, of the protesters. Of utmost importance to keep in mind is that the feeling that is moving the majority of the student protesters is a strong compassion for their fellow human beings who are suffering so much in Gaza. The compassion felt by these protesters includes the feeling of such abhorrence for the slaughter they see going on in Gaza that they must...